EDITORIAL
EDITORIAL
Introduction to a special issue on the neuroscience of motivation and emotion
Eddie Harmon-Jones • Jack van Honk
Published online: 28 February 2012
� Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
Neuroscience methods have gained widespread use in
many areas of psychological and behavioral sciences. Their
use has likewise increased in research and theory on
motivation and emotion. As a consequence of this explo-
sion of interest, we organized this special issue. In orga-
nizing this issue, we thought it important to have
contributions from a range of neuroscience methods,
because addressing issues concerning motivation and
emotion requires multiple methods, each with their own
benefits and limitations, particularly when used in studying
emotive processes in humans. In addition, we invited sci-
entists to contribute who are making programmatic neu-
roscientific contributions to the study of motivation and
emotion, and were pleased that all invited agreed to
contribute.
The study of physiological and biological processes in
motivation and emotion dates back several decades in non-
human animal research and human research. Indeed, sci-
entists studying motivation and emotion were some of the
first to employ physiological measurements in their work.
Consider Walter Cannon’s (1915) classic experiments on
the fight or flight response, and Albert Ax’s (1953) classic
experiment elucidating physiological differences between
anger and fear. However, different names of the
field(s) employing these methods and models have been
used over time. The field has been referred to as biological
psychology, psychobiology, physiological psychology,
neuropsychology, psychophysiology, as well as other
names. Although some of these terms are still used to
describe specific types of conceptual and/or methodologi-
cal approaches, in our view, these fields are all associated
with neuroscience, the term most commonly used today in
describing research into physiological and biological pro-
cesses involving brain/mind.
The explosion of interest in neuroscience within the
study of motivation, emotion, and other psychological
processes is likely due to several factors. One prominent
factor contributed to the explosion of interest is the advent
of new and more readily available methods. For example,
the signal of most interest in functional magnetic resonance
imaging, the blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD)
signal, was first successfully measured in the human brain
in the early 1990s (Kwong et al. 1992), and it has since
gained widespread research usage. Also, within the last
decade, several companies produced easy-to-use acquisi-
tion and signal processing systems for use with physio-
logical responses such as electroencephalography (EEG),
event-related brain potentials (ERPs), and electromyogra-
phy. At the same time, the growing interest in the neuro-
science of human motivation and emotion coincided with
the advent of salivary measurement and methods for acute
single administrations of the hormones oxytocin and tes-
tosterone, and in research these hormones subsequently
proved to be critical motivators for numerous human social
and affective behaviors (Bos et al. in press).
Emotive neuroscience integrates diverse literatures,
theories, and methodologies to address questions about
brain, mind, and behavior and thus creates a science
E. Harmon-Jones (&) University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
e-mail: eddiehj@gmail.com
J. van Honk
Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
J. van Honk
Cape Town University, Cape Town, South Africa
123
Motiv Emot (2012) 36:1–3
DOI 10.1007/s11031-012-9281-x
presumably closer to yielding answers to important ques-
tions. This interdisciplinary research approach can poten-
tially provide a bridge to other knowledge that may
ultimately help to explain or better understand a condition
or behavior. For example, activity in a brain region may be
associated with other behaviors, neurotransmitters, or
hormones that may in turn might shed light on the original
behavior or condition of interest (Carver and Harmon-
Jones 2009). Also, neuroscience methods can be used in
tests of theoretical interpretations of behavioral effects, as
several papers in this special issue illustrate.
In addition, neuroscience methods provide sources of
information that subvert some of the problems with self-
report and other behavioral measures. For example, mea-
sures of brain activity, such as EEG, ERP, and fMRI, allow
researchers to record rapid, online changes in motivational
and affective responses that would otherwise be impossible
to assess without interrupting a participant’s engagement in
an experimental manipulation or impossible to assess
because these processes are not available in consciousness.
As noted above, the contributors, who are making
important programmatic neuroscientific contributions to
the study of motivation and emotion, employ a range of
neuroscience methods and address a range of questions
related to motivation and emotion. We briefly review their
contributions below.
Jaak Panksepp, a pioneer in the field who coined the
term affective neuroscience, leads off with an essay
pointing to the necessity of studying primary emotional
feelings in non-humans. He differentiates these processes
from secondary-processes concerning learning and mem-
ory and tertiary processes concerning cognitive thinking
and rumination. In the end, he posits that this conceptual
view integrates basic and dimensional approaches to
emotions.
Alexander Todorov reviews findings from primate and
human neuroscience on face processing and the amygdala.
He considers faces as imperative stimuli, and suggests that
cognition, affect and motivation intersect in face percep-
tion. One of the key functions of the amygdala would be to
direct attention to faces that are atypical or ambiguous.
Todorov’s in-depth framework is also consistent with
amygdala findings that do not involve faces, and is there-
fore a universal account for the role of the amygdala in
perception.
Tom Price and Eddie Harmon-Jones review research
suggesting that manipulated facial expressions, hand con-
tractions, and changes in physical posture influence
approach motivation or the inclination to move toward a
stimulus as assessed by physiological measures (i.e.,
asymmetric EEG alpha power over the frontal cortex, the
late positive potential of the ERP, and the startle eyeblink
response). They conclude that bi-directionality may exist
between certain bodily movements and other components
of approach- or avoidance-related emotions.
Jennifer Beer reviews recent research on motivated
social cognition. Her review reveals what can be learned by
examining motivational influences on the neural systems
underlying social cognition. In particular, her review sug-
gests that unrealistically positive evaluations of oneself and
one’s close other causes reduced orbitofrontal cortex acti-
vation compared to evaluations of others. Thus, these
results contribute to the debate over whether unrealistic
positivity reflects active distortion or cognitive conserva-
tion and they are more consistent with the cognitive miser
perspective.
Dennis Schutter and Gennady Knyazev expertly review
electrophysiological studies on the relations between
motivation, emotion and cross-frequency coupling of brain
oscillations. They suggest, on basis of this evidence, that
the study of interdependencies of brain oscillations may be
a valuable approach for studying processes associated with
motivation and emotion. For instance, amplitude–ampli-
tude coupling between delta-alpha and delta-beta is asso-
ciated with state anxiety and approach-avoidance-related
motivation. Also, the coupling of delta-beta oscillations
changes following successful psychotherapy.
John Jost and David Amodio provide a timely review
integrating previous behavioural research on motives
underlying political orientation with emotive neuroscience
research concerned with reactions to uncertainty, ambigu-
ity, threat, and disgust. Their review suggests that right-
(vs. left-) wing political orientation is associated with
greater neural sensitivity to threat and a larger amygdala
region, as well as less sensitivity to response conflict and a
smaller anterior cingulate region.
Estrella Montoya, David Terburg, Peter Bos, and Jack
van Honk put forward a framework for the interactive role
of steroid hormones cortisol and testosterone and the
monoamine serotonin in impulsive aggression. First
focusing on steroid hormones, they review evidence that
suggests that high testosterone-to-cortisol ratio sets a pre-
disposition for social aggressive behaviour in general.
Next, they review evidence that suggests that serotonin
may differentiate between impulsive and instrumental
aggression, in that low prefrontal serotonin synthesis in
combination with a high testosterone-low cortisol ratio
produces a socially explosive mind.
Alicia Salvador in her insightful review discusses the
interactive role of the hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal
(HPG) axis and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA)
axis in a broad spectrum of social behaviors. The interac-
tions between these axes and their end products, the hor-
mones testosterone and cortisol, are highly adaptive in
social situations that involve competition and challenge
and have components of social stress.
2 Motiv Emot (2012) 36:1–3
123
Anna Weinberg, Anja Riesel, and Greg Hajcak review
over two decades of research and theories about the error-
related negativity (ERN), a negative-going wave in the
event-related brain potential that occurs following the
commission of an error. After presenting and critically
evaluating cognitive theories of the ERN, they review
research that suggests that the ERN is neural index of a
neurobehavioral trait and variation in its amplitude is
partially related to individual differences in defensive
reactivity.
Collectively, these papers illustrate the multifarious
ways in which the inclusion of neuroscience can benefit the
study of motivation and emotion. They demonstrate how
neuroscience approaches can lead to better understandings
of phenomena; generate new predictions and new theories,
even at the behavioural level; inform established psycho-
logical theories; and be used in tests of competing theories.
References
Ax, A. F. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and
anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433–442. Bos, P. A., Panksepp, J., Bluthé, R.-M., & van Honk, J. (in press).
Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human
social—emotional behavior: A review of single administration
studies. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. Cannon, W. B. (1915). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage:
An account of recent researches into the function of emotional excitement. New York, NY: D. Appleton & Company.
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-
related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulle- tin, 135, 183–204.
Kwong, K. K., Belliveau, J. W., Chesler, D. A., Goldberg, I. E.,
Weisskoff, R. M., Poncelet, B. P., et al. (1992). Dynamic
magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during
primary sensory stimulation. Proceedings of National Academy of Science USA, 89, 5675–5679.
Motiv Emot (2012) 36:1–3 3
123
Copyright of Motivation & Emotion is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder’s express
written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.